Using variation to understand the grammar of Basque Dative Displacement

نویسنده

  • Aaron Ecay
چکیده

Aaron Ecay Introduction The morphology of the Basque auxiliary verb is very rich, comprising morphemes which spell out features of tense and mood, as well as the φ-features of up to three verbal arguments. This morphology has been the subject of much study, however the interaction between variation and formal analysis has been largely ignored. In this paper I demonstrate that there are rich systematic aspects of morphosyntactic dialect variation in Basque, that this variation calls into question previous formal analyses, and that it can inform the creation of new ones. Dative displacement Dative Displacement (DD; ex. 1) is a change in Basque morphology whereby a dative object concords with typically a absolutive prefix. Figure 1 illustrates the distribution of this change within Basque dialects: there is a cohesive region of DD in the north, but also several isolated spots where it has developed independently. Pluralizers Basque auxiliary verbs can contain the morphemes zki and it, often seen as markers of plural number of the absolutive (2, 3). Arregi & Nevins (2012) argue that it is a reflex of Agree spelled out on T; Preminger (2009) gives a similar though less detailed account. However, data from DD dialects demonstrate that the story is more complicated. In the dialect of Irun, the usual ditransitive pluralizer is it, generating forms such as the one on the left-hand side of example (4). However, when optional DD applies to a 1sg dative, the pluralizer becomes zki – the only case in which zki is seen in ditransitive auxiliaries. In fact, no dialect combines it as ditransitive pluralizer with DD. This distributional fact suggests that it is stem allomorphy triggered by the prefix, and not a reflex of Agree.1 This conclusion is bolstered by considering dialects such as that of Ahetze, where the auxiliary may contain both it and zki: in this case, it is zki which marks the plurality of the absolutive, and it which corresponds to the prefixal dative. Metathetic processes Arregi & Nevins (2012) predict that if DD is realized by doubling of the dative marker in rather than total replacement of a suffix by a prefix, the dative cannot influence the form of T (the auxiliary stem) because this replacement is necessarily computed after T Agrees; DD-by-replacement may be computed before or after Agree (5.161). This analysis is disconfirmed in the dialect of Urdiain, which contains the form in (6), where the dative prefix doubles a suffixal dative marker, yet the stem allomorphy is influenced by the person features thereof.2 For A&N the difference between DD-doubling and DD-replacement fundamentally involves where in the grammar they are computed. However, DD-doubling is rare compared to DD-replacement, and always co-occurs with variation. The dialect of Sara, which lacked DD in the mid-1800s but is in the process of acquiring it, has variable DD-doubling with plural absolutives, but categorical DD-replacement with singulars. This indicates that DD has taken hold more quickly in the singular than the plural paradigm, as also observed in other dialects which are acquiringDD.DD-doubling is thus an intermediate step in the acquisition of DD; whether there are two possible loci for DD-type rules in the grammar (as A&N propose) is orthogonal to the progress of DD as a change, as the data from Urdiain demonstrate. Conditioning factors on variation Some authors have observed that dative displacement depends on the person featuers of the dative argument (Rezac 2008), and in some cases have written such dependence into their analyses (Arregi and Nevins 2012; ch. 5 (150)). However, the dialects with this dependence are on the periphery of the northern DD region, or are one of the isolated DD dialects. Dialects from the center of the northern DD region have DD for all non-3rd person datives. Similarly, Rezac & Fernandez 2013 describe a taxonomy of “systematic factors” which condition the availability of DD, versus factors which are excluded from doing so by their analysis. However, their predictions about factors which do not condition DD (ergative and allocutive φ-features) each find dialects which counterexemplify them (Espelette, where DD is only available for a 1sg dative with 3sg ergative, but not other ergatives; Vera and Lezo, where DD is only available with non-allocutive forms).3 Consideration of the spatial structure of variation raises the question of whether surface patterns of inconsistency in one locality’s dialect are due to grammatical rules that are specific to certain φ-feature bundles, or are the result of a maximally general rule which is spreading across space and time at different rates in different contexts. It also gives one pause to rely on conditioning factors in ongoing variation to undergird a non-variational analysis of the phenomenon. Conclusion The structure of this variation in geographic space, time, and grammatical feature geometry is vital to arriving at a correct synchronic, single-dialect linguistic analysis. At a minimum the reification of the distribution of variation into the grammar must be avoided; however facts about variation can also suggest novel analyses as in the case of differentiating it from other pluralizers.

برای دانلود رایگان متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

منابع مشابه

Dative Overmarking in Basque: Evidence of Spanish-Basque Convergence

This paper investigates a recent change in the grammar of spoken Basque which results in the substitution of dative case and agreement for absolutive case and agreement in marking animate, specific direct objects. I argue that this pattern of use is due to convergence between the feature matrix of the functional category AGR in Spanish and Basque. In contrast to previous studies which point to ...

متن کامل

Basque clitics in morphosyntax*

This paper addresses the question of φ -feature marking in Basque. Basque finite verbs display an uncommonly large number of morphemes which covary with the φ -features of the absolutive, ergative, and dative arguments as well as (in some dialects) the gender of the addressee. Recent work (Preminger 2009; Arregi & Nevins 2012) has addressed the question of how these morphemes are derived. In ta...

متن کامل

A Working Report on Statistically Modeling Dative Variation in Mandarin Chinese

Dative variation is a widely observed syntactic phenomenon in world languages (e.g. I gave John a book and I gave a book to John). It has been shown that which surface form will be used in a dative sentence is not a completely random choice, rather, it is conditioned by a wide range of linguistic factors. Previous work by Bresnan and colleagues adopted a statistical modeling approach to investi...

متن کامل

Score Variation in Multiple-Choice Tests of Grammar: On the Effect of Gender and Stem Type

This paper examines the effect of gender and type of stem on Iranian test takers’ performance on the grammar section of a nation-wide language proficiency test. In so doing, the scores of 2931 examinees (1107 female and 1824 male) who sat Tarbiat Modares English proficiency test were obtained. The examinees’ scores on three types of MC grammar items with different kinds of stems (i.e., blank fi...

متن کامل

Thematic Volume on Bare Nominal 11.1. Edições Colibri. [Pre-‐final version] When quantifiers do not agree: three systems

Basque weak quantifiers optionally agree with the inflected verb in number. This papers’ main aim is to study the dialectal variation shown by this phenomenon. The study will show that it is necessary to differentiate at least three systems: the western-central system, one that we will call the transition system, and the eastern system (souletin). The western-central system allows the presence ...

متن کامل

ذخیره در منابع من


  با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید

عنوان ژورنال:

دوره   شماره 

صفحات  -

تاریخ انتشار 2014